Do you remember the name of your second-grade teacher or what you ate for lunch today? Those memories may be separated by decades, but both are considered long-term memories.
More than half a century ago, neuroscientists discovered that damage to a brain region called the medial temporal lobe (MTL) caused a severe impairment to long-term declarative memory — memories for explicit facts such as names and dates — but left very short-term memory intact. Patients with damage to the MTL could keep up with and carry on a short conversation but, just a minute or two later, couldn’t remember that the conversation even took place. Surprisingly, though, those patients could learn new motor skills and retain them for days, months, or even longer, indicating that MTL damage had little effect on memories for motor skills.
Long-Term Motor Skill Memory
So, what brain region is responsible for long-term motor skill memories, like riding a bike? Are there distinct regions where short- and long-term sensorimotor memories are formed? Researchers have been trying to answer these questions for years. Now, researchers from the Harvard John A. Paulson School of Engineering and Applied Sciences (SEAS) have shown that, just like declarative memories, short-term and long-term memories for motor skills form in different regions of the brain, with the cerebellum being critical for the formation of long-term skill memories. The research is published in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS).
“This work advances our understanding of the role of the cerebellum in sensorimotor learning and points towards the role of the cerebellum as a gateway to the formation of stable memories for sensorimotor skills, largely independent of the short-term memory systems,” said Maurice Smith, Gordon McKay Professor of Bioengineering at SEAS and senior author of the study.
Researchers have long known that the cerebellum is critical for motor learning, but the role it plays in forming short- and longer-term skill memory was unclear. To understand the connection between the cerebellum and these memories, Smith and first author Alkis Hadjiosif, a postdoctoral fellow at SEAS and Massachusetts General Hospital, took inspiration from a seemingly messy set of previous results on motor learning in patients with cerebellar damage.
While these previous studies all found evidence for impaired sensorimotor learning in individuals with cerebellar damage, the size of this impairment varied widely among them. “While this discrepancy might have been due to differences in the amount or precise location of the damage or to differences in the types of motor learning tasks employed, we had a different idea,” said Smith.
The Importance of Time
Smith and Hadjiosif thought that subtle differences in the time between trials — what they call the memory window — might explain most of the observed discrepancies. “This would be the case if long-term sensorimotor memory was specifically impaired by cerebellar damage because longer memory windows would increase reliance on the impaired long-term memory,” said Hadjiosif. The challenge was that these time intervals were seldom reported in published papers. Part researchers, part detectives, Smith and Hadjiosif tracked down the detailed raw data from two of these studies, from which they could determine the inter-trial intervals for the entire trial sequences for all the individuals studied.
The researchers found that both studies had rather short inter-trial intervals overall and reported only small impairments in learning for patients with severe cerebellar disease compared to healthy individuals. This meant that when participants were asked to perform the same task, say, five times, with only a few seconds between each repetition, the patients with cerebellar degeneration performed only slightly worse than healthy individuals.
But by diving deeper into the data, Smith and Hadjiosif found something interesting. Between trials, there was sometimes more time to allow the research team to reset or the participant to take a short break. “When we examined these trial-to-trial differences, we found that the same patients who displayed near-normal performance on their short-interval practice trials were dramatically impaired on long-interval trials within the same session. And this was the case in the data from both studies,” said Hadjiosif. The team then looked at more than a dozen additional studies in which individuals with cerebellar degeneration performed motor tasks and found that the studies that used a larger number of movement directions in the task —which would increase the time between same-direction trials that would share sensorimotor memory — had dramatically increased memory impairment compared to those with fewer movement directions.
“These findings highlight how important time is to understanding memory degradation in patients with cerebellar degeneration and solve the mystery of the trial-to-trial and study-to-study variability in the effects of cerebellar damage on sensorimotor learning ability,” said Smith. “Our research usually involves designing new experimental manipulations to acquire novel data sets that can provide insight into the mechanisms for learning and memory, but sometimes simply looking at old data through the right lens can be even more illuminating.”
StepUp Note
This research reminds us of the importance of movement sequences in learning. The cerebellum is the primary brain area which coordinates movement sequences. StepUp to Learn programs use rhythmic movement sequences to increase our focused attention for fluent academic learning of basic skills, including reading decoding, alphabet knowledge and math facts.
Note by Nancy W Rowe, MS, CCC/A
Reposted from Harvard University